How best to use Ocular-Motor Deception Detection (ODT) to Discover Sexual Cheating

Ocular-Motor Deception Detection (ODT) technology is an emerging tool in lie detection that leverages eye movements and pupil dilation to identify deceptive behaviors. It operates on the premise that lying requires more cognitive effort than truth-telling, which manifests in subtle ocular changes. While traditionally applied in criminal investigations and security screenings, ODT has the potential to be used in interpersonal relationships, such as in cases where individuals suspect sexual infidelity. This essay explores how ODT technology might be applied to discover sexual cheating, its benefits, challenges, and ethical considerations.
Updated on

Introduction

Ocular-Motor Deception Detection (ODT) technology is an emerging tool in lie detection that leverages eye movements and pupil dilation to identify deceptive behaviors. It operates on the premise that lying requires more cognitive effort than truth-telling, which manifests in subtle ocular changes. While traditionally applied in criminal investigations and security screenings, ODT has the potential to be used in interpersonal relationships, such as in cases where individuals suspect sexual infidelity. This essay explores how ODT technology might be applied to discover sexual cheating, its benefits, challenges, and ethical considerations.

 

The Science Behind ODT


ODT measures specific involuntary ocular responses that arise from cognitive load during deception. When a person is lying, they typically exhibit increased pupil dilation, longer response times, and more frequent blinking due to the cognitive effort involved in fabricating a story or withholding the truth. Pupil dilation, in particular, has been linked to mental strain, as deception requires more brain resources to manage inconsistencies and maintain control over the lie.


A study by Cook et al. (2012) found that ODT could detect deception with an accuracy rate of 85%. This technology works by asking individuals a series of questions, some of which are neutral and others designed to provoke a cognitive load, depending on the individual’s guilt or innocence. When applied to discovering sexual cheating, the system could potentially be adapted to analyze responses to questions related to the subject's fidelity, relationship history, or specific events that could reveal infidelity.

 

Application of ODT to Detect Sexual Cheating


In a scenario where one partner suspects sexual infidelity, ODT could be employed to monitor the ocular responses of the accused partner when answering questions related to fidelity. For instance, an ODT examination might include questions about the person’s whereabouts during a suspected affair, their interactions with the person they are allegedly cheating with, and the specifics of their sexual and emotional behavior in the relationship.


During this examination, the suspect would be exposed to both comparison questions (e.g., questions about activities the person has likely not done) and critical questions (e.g., about the alleged affair). Ocular responses to these critical questions, particularly increased pupil dilation and delayed responses, might suggest deception. Because the system detects subtle, involuntary physiological changes, it would be harder for someone to manipulate their responses or conceal guilt.

 

Benefits of ODT relative to Polygraph in Relationship Investigations


One of the main advantages of using ODT to uncover sexual infidelity is its non-invasive nature. Unlike polygraphs, which require the subject to be hooked up to various sensors, ODT simply monitors the eyes, making it less intimidating for the person being tested. Additionally, ODT measures cognitive effort rather than physiological arousal, reducing the chances of false positives caused by anxiety, stress, or nervousness unrelated to the subject matter.


ODT also offers a level of objectivity that could help resolve disputes where one partner accuses the other without substantial evidence. By providing data-driven insights, ODT can reduce the emotional biases often present in confrontations about infidelity.

 

What Kind of Questions can be Asked?


Like all lie detection and truth verification technologies, the test works best on questions of behavior rather than questions of feelings, desires or intentions. For example, a question of “do you love me?” is much less useful than a question of “have you had sexual contact with anyone else?” One is a feeling, the other is a behavior. Lying about feelings is difficult because they are so undefined and fleeting. How many of us have at one moment felt we loved someone or something and then in the next minute changed our minds? 


Ultimately, the best questions are “action” questions that we can ask whether the person did them or not. 

 

What if a person fails a test?


Remember, it’s only one data point among many. The first opportunity is to get a confession. Using the failed test as an indication of lying, then eliciting a confession is the first step.


If the person refuses to admit wrongdoing, the next step is to combine the test results with other data, which will either strengthen or weaken the case.


Finally, it is possible an innocent person is flagged as guilty. The test is 85% accurate, so by definition, some cases will be coded incorrectly. Other reasons an innocent person might fail include: 

  • Is a victim of or witness to a serious crime or act
  • Has committed a similar crime or act
  • Has enabled someone else or facilitated the crime or act
  • Is covering for someone involved in a crime or act
  • While testing, the person is worried about another serious crime
  • Misunderstands the test questions
  • Was intimidated by external circumstances before or during testing
  • Was intoxicated beyond an ability to understand and give answers

 

Ethical Concerns and Limitations

Despite its advantages, using ODT in personal relationships, particularly to discover sexual cheating, raises significant ethical concerns. First, the technology could violate privacy if used without the explicit consent of the person being tested. The idea of “mind-reading” through physiological signals might also lead to misinterpretations of the data. Not every deceptive response necessarily points to cheating; a person could experience cognitive load when faced with uncomfortable or emotionally charged questions even if they are not guilty of infidelity.


Additionally, the technology’s accuracy, while promising, is not perfect. The 85% accuracy rate suggests that there is still room for error, which could lead to false accusations or wrongful assumptions. This could have devastating effects on relationships, leading to breakups or emotional harm if the technology is misused or the results are overinterpreted.


One recommended approach is to use any lie detection score as only one data point among many. Usually there is a confirmation of guilt arising from many different data sources.

 

Conclusion


ODT technology offers a novel and potentially effective method for detecting sexual cheating by monitoring involuntary eye movements and cognitive load. Its non-invasive nature and ability to measure cognitive effort make it a valuable tool for lie detection. However, its application in personal relationships must be approached with caution, considering the ethical implications and potential for misuse. While ODT could provide objective insights into suspicions of infidelity, trust and communication remain the cornerstone of resolving issues in relationships, and technology should be seen as a supplement, not a substitute, for these critical components.


References:

  1. Cook, A. E., et al. (2012). "Evaluating the accuracy of an ocular-motor deception test." *Behavior Research Methods*, 44(4), 1007-1016.